usin
B
companies, consumers, money, technology, work
ess
SUNDAY, JULY 21, 2019
Section
D
INSIDE
date­line PAGE D-3   click & Clack PAGE D-8
Questions about delivery or service? Call 1-800-228-NEWS (6397)
E-mail: business@post-gazette.com Web: post-gazette.com/businessnews Editor: Teresa F. Lindeman, 412-263-2018
CLASSIFIEDS
PAGE d-4
FOR MORE BUSINESS COVERAGE AND
BREAKING UPDATES, VISIT POST-GAZETTE.COM
Sometimes, mum’s
the word
in Uber
or Lyft ride
Now you can pay for that silent treatment

There’s one Uber ride, in par­tic­u­lar, that Beth Turn­bull re­calls clear as day. When the 21-year-old Ir­win na­tive was picked up, the driver told her that he was a pro­fes­sional SpongeBob SquarePants im­per­son­ator.

As a reg­u­lar user of ride-hail­ing ser­vices, she def­i­nitely meets some “col­or­ful char­ac­ters” that she doesn’t mind chat­ting with, said Ms. Turn­bull, an ac­counts co­or­di­na­tor at Mat­ter Com­mu­ni­ca­tions, a pub­lic re­la­tions firm in the Strip Dis­trict. But most of the time, she pre­fers si­lence in the car.

She’s not the only one.

San Fran­cisco-based Uber rolled out a new fea­ture in May, aptly named “quiet mode.” It’s a new ser­vice that al­lows rid­ers to re­quest a no-talk­ing ex­pe­ri­ence.

Uber did not re­turn a re­quest for com­ment for this story.

Quiet mode is avail­able in ev­ery city, in­clud­ing Pitts­burgh, al­though only to those us­ing Uber Black and Uber Black SUV — pre­mium ser­vices that rou­tinely cost dou­ble the price of a reg­u­lar ride.

It’s a sharp con­trast from the ear­li­est days of Uber in the early 2010s, when rid­ers were en­cour­aged to sit up front; it was the anti-taxi ser­vice, af­ter all. How­ever, that close prox­im­ity to the driver made con­ver­sa­tion a ne­ces­sity, more or less.

Many rid­ers in Pitts­burgh choose to sit in the back seat now, a way to help get a si­lent ride.

And that’s good for Uber be­cause the in-app quiet mode may cre­ate an in­cen­tive for cus­tom­ers to up­grade to a more ex­pen­sive Uber Black ve­hi­cle.

‘More than a hired hand’

Kwadwo Burgee, lead an­a­lyst for the Depart­ment of Home­land Se­cu­rity in Wash­ing­ton, D.C., has no in­ter­est in Uber’s quiet mode ser­vice.

For one thing, the 44-year-old Arling­ton, Va., res­i­dent — orig­i­nally from Phil­a­del­phia — said he was raised to keep quiet dur­ing taxi rides.

And why pay ex­tra?

By Courtney Linder
Pittsburgh Post-Gazette
SEE RIDE, PAGE D-2
A HARD LESSON
Retiree says she wasn’t told that annuity would tie up her investment
Darrell Sapp/Post-Gazette
Deborah Clark, 68, in her living room in the Arlington section of Pittsburgh.

Deb­o­rah Clark knew noth­ing about in­vest­ing when she re­tired four years ago from UPMC.

The only thing she knew for sure was that she never wanted to worry about her health in­sur­ance be­ing paid and that she wanted the money in her re­tire­ment ac­count to gen­er­ate enough in­come to cover the monthly pre­mium through her re­tire­ment years.

“That was the most im­por­tant thing to me,” said the 68-year-old Arling­ton woman.

Her plan was vi­a­ble enough. She had saved $107,000 in her em­ployee re­tire­ment ac­count over 36 years work­ing as an ad­min­is­tra­tor. She wanted to roll the money into a tra­di­tional IRA. She dis­cussed the plan with the PNC Bank man­ager at UPMC Pres­by­te­rian hos­pi­tal where she worked, and the man­ager ar­ranged for Ms. Clark to meet with a bank in­vest­ment ad­viser.

She re­mem­bers the meet­ing be­ing brief and to the point. The in­vest­ment ad­viser made one

tele­phone call to ask some­one how much money was needed to gen­er­ate a monthly in­come stream of $242. He hung up the phone and told her the magic num­ber: $65,475.

“There was no dis­cus­sion what­so­ever,” Ms. Clark said. “But I was com­fort­able with the fact that my health in­sur­ance was be­ing cov­ered.”

She would later find out that the in­vest­ment ad­viser rolled her re­tire­ment ac­count into a guar­an­teed life­time an­nu­ity. The an­nu­ity would pay her $299 a month for the rest of her life. But she would no lon­ger have ac­cess to the $65,475 she in­vested.

By the time Ms. Clark re­al­ized she could never with­draw any of

the money she gave the ad­viser, she said she had al­ready spent the funds she had left over — $10,000 on pre­paid fu­neral ar­range­ments for her­self, $8,000 to bury her sis­ter, and $20,000 on home ren­o­va­tions and travel.

She be­lieves she was mis­led to think her money was in a tra­di­tional stocks-and-bonds IRA that al­lowed with­draw­als.

Mean­while, her three-bed­room home still needs work to stop flood­ing in the base­ment. She has a pay­ment of $540 a month on the 30-year mort­gage she took on in 2004. She pays her bills with a $1,440 monthly So­cial Se­cu­rity check. She has no emer­gency sav­ings.

“It’s very up­set­ting,” she said.

PNC Bank does not be­lieve Ms. Clark was mis­led in the an­nu­ity pur­chase. A spokes­woman for PNC Bank, Mar­cey Zwiebel, would not dis­cuss the case in de­tail. She pro­vided a writ­ten state­ment ex­press­ing the bank’s po­si­tion.

“The firm thor­oughly in­ves­ti­gated the com­plaint in ques­tion and de­ter­mined that its fi­nan­cial ad­viser acted ap­pro­pri­ately con­cern­ing the in­vest­ment,” she said.

In­ter­est in an­nu­i­ties

The an­nu­i­ties mar­ket has been gain­ing mo­men­tum in re­cent years, driven pri­mar­ily by baby boomers look­ing to avoid stock mar­ket risk while seek­ing a guar­an­teed monthly in­come stream.

An an­nu­ity is not an in­vest­ment in stocks and bonds. It is a con­tract with an in­sur­ance com­pany, which is why only li­censed in­sur­ance agents are al­lowed to sell them.

There are sev­eral kinds of an­nu­i­ties. Es­sen­tially, the con­sumer makes ei­ther a sin­gle pay­ment or

By Tim Grant
Pittsburgh Post-Gazette
SEE ANNUITY, PAGE D-2

“Se­nior cit­i­zens like my­self want to live a fi­nan­cial and com­fort­able life. I would not have agreed to hav­ing an in­come for my health cov­er­age and not have ac­cess for fi­nan­cial emer­gency for five years.”

Deb­o­rah Clark, in a let­ter
to New York Life

Poker-playing AI figures out how to call their bluffs

Dar­ren Elias makes a liv­ing play­ing poker, to the tune of $7 mil­lion in tour­na­ment win­nings as of 2019. He also holds the record for the most World Poker Tour ti­tles, with four to his name.

That makes him one of the most fa­mous play­ers out there. But he says there’s one com­pet­i­tor, in par­tic­u­lar, that he wouldn’t hedge his life’s earn­ings against.

Pluri­bus is the name — and this is one Texas hold’em champ that will quickly snatch up all of your chips. That’s be­cause it’s an ar­ti­fi­cially in­tel­li­gent com­puter pro­gram de­signed to win.

Rep­re­sent­ing a sig­nifi­cant leap in AI tech, Pluri­bus has the abil­ity to beat five hu­man poker play­ers, at once.

It was de­vel­oped by Tuo­mas Sand­holm, a pro­fes­sor of com­puter sci­ence at Car­ne­gie Mel­lon Univer­sity, and Noam Brown, a re­search sci­en­tist at Face­book AI in New York City, who is also wrap­ping up his Ph.D in com­puter sci­ence at CMU.

Their aim is not to rev­o­lu­tion­ize poker, but to use it as a mile­stone for prog­ress in AI re­search. “Using games as test beds gives you a way to score how strong the AI is,” said Mr. Brown.

Mr. Elias, 32 of Med­ford, N.J.,

said, as a poker ex­pert, he helped the re­search­ers find weak­nesses in Pluri­bus’s per­for­mance in the sys­tem’s ear­li­est days of train­ing. He’d play against five ver­sions of Pluri­bus on­line for five or six hours per day over a one-week pe­riod.

They quickly cleaned up the code when Mr. Elias made sug­ges­tions. What he saw there­af­ter shocked him.

“It would be a lot bet­ter the next day, which is kind of creepy,” he said. “It went from be­ing a me­di­o­cre player to a world-class player in a mat­ter of weeks.”

Not the first, but the last

The re­search on poker and AI has been on­go­ing for some 16 years. Mr. Brown has been in­volved with such proj­ects since 2012, he said.

The work on Pluri­bus was funded by the Na­tional Science Foun­da­tion in Wash­ing­ton, D.C., and the U.S. Army Re­search Of­fice in Durham County, N.C.

By no means is Pluri­bus the first AI sys­tem to beat pro­fes­sional poker play­ers.

In 2017, Mr. Bown and Mr. Sand­holm teamed up on “Li­bra­tus,” an­other AI sys­tem that beat out quite a few play­ers in one-on-one poker matches over a three-week span at the Rivers Ca­sino in the North Shore. It was im­pres­sive, but it only il­lus­trated the strengths of AI against one other com­po­nent.

To ratchet up the stakes, the duo took on a new chal­lenge with Pluri­bus — the name means “many.” Texas hold’em is a six-player game.

Most ex­perts in the AI com­mu­nity, ac­a­demic and in­dus­try, thought Mr. Brown and Mr. Sand­holm could never build a sys­tem like Pluri­bus us­ing their meth­od­ol­ogy. That en­tailed a dif­fer­ent kind of ma­chine learn­ing, a method to teach an AI pro­gram how to func­tion by feed­ing

it in­for­ma­tion, called train­ing data. Once a sys­tem un­der­stands re­la­tion­ships be­tween those pieces of data, it can make pre­dic­tions on its own.

With Pluri­bus, there was no out­side data — mean­ing no in­for­ma­tion from ac­tual Texas hold’em games that hu­mans have played. Rather, the com­puter learned to mas­ter the game by play­ing against it­self and us­ing what it learned to cre­ate its own train­ing data that self-in­forms the AI

mov­ing for­ward to the next game.

Over and over and over.

“It grad­u­ally im­proves over time ... it learns how to play poker by fig­ur­ing out how to beat it­self,” Mr. Brown said.

Now, Pluri­bus can see all op­tions on the ta­ble — lit­er­ally — and come up with an idea of what makes sense at first glance. It’s just like see­ing a chess board and no­tic­ing the first pos­si­ble move that seems log­i­cal.

And it only takes its com­puter brain 10 to 15 sec­onds.

The pris­on­ers’ di­lemma

To come up with that ma­trix of choices, Pluri­bus re­lies on “game the­ory,” an area of study in eco­nom­ics that deals with the dif­fer­ing strat­e­gies and in­cen­tives of mul­ti­ple par­tic­i­pants in com­pet­i­tive sit­u­a­tions. In other words, the out­come of one per­son’s choice is de­pen­dent on the ac­tion of the other per­son.

Game the­ory has been ap­plied in the con­texts of busi­ness, gam­ing, war and even bi­ol­ogy.

The clas­sic ex­am­ple is called the “pris­on­ers’ di­lemma,” wherein two ra­tio­nal in­di­vid­u­als fac­ing crim­i­nal sen­tences might not co­op­er­ate, even if it’s in their best in­ter­est to do so.

Here’s the gist, as de­scribed by Al­bert Wil­liam Tucker, a Ca­na­dian math­e­ma­ti­cian, who for­mal­ized the idea:

By Courtney Linder
Pittsburgh Post-Gazette
Steven Senne
A card dealer runs a game of poker. CMU’s new artificial intelligence system has surprised even the pros with its ability to crack the nuances of Texas hold’em. The AI system could lead to tools for other industries.
SEE POKER, PAGE D-3